Abstract
<p> Executive functions (EF) are neurocognitive processes that allow for goal-oriented behaviors, accommodate responses to new and ambiguous experiences, and regulate one’s ideas, feelings, and behaviors. <sup> 1,2 </sup> EF consists of attention, cognitive flexibility, updating, and inhibition control. The plasticity of these systems is most substantial from three to five years old. <sup> 2,3 </sup> Research has demonstrated that EF in preschool correlates to other significant developmental milestones throughout life (e.g., academic achievement, social cognitive skills, emotional regulation). <sup> 4 </sup> Contrasted with parents from low SES families, parents from high SES families spend more time reading to their children, telling stories to them, and engaging in puzzle play with them. <sup> 5 </sup> Parents from high SES families provide more books, learning resources, learning opportunities, and use private childcare. <sup> 5 </sup> , Research has shown that these actions increase children’s attention, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. <sup> 5 </sup> Compared to monolingual children, bilingual children have increased inhibition, attention shifting, and cognitive flexibility. <sup> 6,7 </sup> Regardless of racial, ethnic, or nationality group, bilingual children consistently demonstrate more advanced inhibitory control compared to monolingual children. <sup> 8 </sup> Household disorganization negatively influences inhibitory control, attention shifting, and working memory. <sup> 1,9 </sup> Household instability correlates to decreased effortful control and inhibition. <sup> 1, 9 </sup> When parents engage in scaffolding and autonomy support in problem-solving scenarios, it improves children’s working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibition. <sup> 2,10 </sup> Children experiencing sensitive and responsive caregiving exhibit better inhibitory control, working memory, and cognitive flexibility. <sup> 9,10,11 </sup> Contrastingly, parents’ regular engagement in negative controlling behaviors (e.g., being overly strict) leads to children’s decreases in inhibition. <sup> 10,11 </sup></p><p> <strong> References </strong> <ol> <li> Andrews, K., Atkinson, L., Harris, M., & Gonzalez, A. (2021a). Examining the effects of household chaos on child executive functions: A meta-analysis. <em> Psychological Bulletin, 147 </em> (1), 16-32. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000311 </li> <li> Fay-Stammbach, T., Hawes, D. J., & Meredith, P. (2014). Parenting influences on executive function in early childhood: A review. <em> Child Development Perspectives, 8 </em> (4), 258-264. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12095 </li> <li> Yang, X., Chung, K. K. H., & McBride, C. (2019). Longitudinal contributions of executive functioning and visual-spatial skills to mathematics learning in young Chinese children. <em> Educational Psychology, 39 </em> , 678-704. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2018.1546831 </li> <li> Baptista, J., Osório, A., Martins, E. C., Castiajo, P., Barreto, A. L., Mateus, V., Soares, I., & Martins, C. (2016). Maternal and paternal mental-state talk and executive function in preschool children. <em> Social Development, 26 </em> (1), 129-145. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12183 </li> <li> Conway, A., Waldfogel, J., & Wang, Y. (2019). Disparities in Kindergarteners’ executive functions at Kindergarten Entry: Relations with parenting and child care. <em> Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 48 </em> , 267-283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.03.009 </li> <li> Bialystok, E. (2010). Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. <em> Developmental Psychology, 46 </em> (1), 93. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015466 </li> <li> Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. <em> Cognition, 112 </em> (3), 494-500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014 </li> <li> Roos, L. E., Beauchamp, K. G., Flannery, J., & Fisher, P. A. (2017). Cultural contributions to childhood executive function. <em> Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8 </em> , 61. </li> <li> Andrews, K., Dunn, J. R., Prime, H., Duku, E., Atkinson, L., Tiwari, A., & Gonzalez, A. (2021b). Effects of household chaos and parental responsiveness on child executive functions: A novel, multi-method approach. <em> BMC Psychology, 9 </em> (1), 147. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-021-00651-1 </li> <li> Kao, K., Nayak, S., Doan, S. N., & Tarullo, A. R. (2018). Relations between parent EF and child EF: The role of socioeconomic status and parenting on executive functioning in early childhood. <em> Translational Issues in Psychological Science, 4 </em> (2), 122-137. https://doi.org/10.1037/tps0000154 </li> <li> Valcan, D. S., Davis, H., & Pino-Pasternak, D. (2018). Parental behaviours predicting early childhood executive functions: A meta-analysis. <em> Educational Psychology Review, 30 </em> (3), 607-649. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-017-9411-9 </li> </ol></p>
Original language | American English |
---|---|
State | Published - 12 Apr 2023 |